Understanding Pain through Science and Simulation

Liz Losin in mock doctor's office with mock patient, simulating a pain perception study

Understanding Pain through Science and Simulation

A UM psychology professor studies the perception and communication of pain in a simulated clinical setting at the College of Arts and Sciences.
A UM psychology professor studies the perception and communication of pain in a simulated clinical setting at the College of Arts and Sciences.
by Jessica M. Castillo
UM News

Social and cultural neuroscientist Elizabeth Losin conducts and directs her lab’s research armed with a simulated clinical setting—a recreated doctor’s office—an MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) brain scanner, and a small wand with a tiny ceramic plate that heats up in calibrated increments to experimentally induce and study pain.

“Because pain, by definition, is a subjective phenomenon, it’s very hard to objectively measure the effects of the painful stimulus,” said Losin, director of the Social and Cultural Neuroscience Lab and assistant professor of psychology at the College of Arts and Sciences at the University of Miami. “In our case, we know the intensity of the painful stimulus because we set it. We make it the same for everyone so we know that any variability we’re seeing is due to things about the person or the environment, not the painful stimulus, for example.”

Flanked by a team of two graduate students, two undergraduates and a full-time research coordinator, Losin works to better understand how cultural processes influence brain function and vice versa.

She studies the mechanisms underlying demographic disparities related to the pain experience, including the perception of pain and pain treatment, couched in the particular context of a clinical setting and doctor-patient interaction. For example, she said, women tend to report more pain than men, and members of certain minority groups tend to report more pain than members of the majority. It’s also been well documented in the research literature, Losin said, that it takes longer for members of minority groups to be prescribed pain medication, and they’re likelier to receive lower doses.

Losin and her team distill a host of factors that could affect these mechanisms and disparities.

“We’re going about understanding these pain disparities from several different angles: one is what happened to the person between when they were born and when they encounter the painful event; the second is what happens in the doctor’s office; and the third is the clinician’s perspective and how that affects pain and pain treatment,” Losin said.

Previous studies focusing on clinician demographics have focused on their gender and how that affects, consciously or subconsciously, the pain experience of the patient. But, “the effect of ethnic or racial concordance between the clinician and the patient on the pain reported by the patient, that has not been as well studied, but it’s part of what we’re looking at,” Losin added.



More Stories

Robots playing soccer
RoboCanes
The University of Miami team of soccer-playing robots engages computer science students to experience hands-on learning in robotics artificial intelligence research.
Active Classroom
Transforming 21st Century Engineering Education
The University of Miami College of Engineering implements an innovative alternative to the traditional lecture format for several of its classes—what's known as an active classroom.

Studying the Brain

Elizabeth Losin, director of the Social and Cultural Neuroscience Lab and assistant professor of psychology at UM, helps reduce health disparities by gaining a fuller understanding of the brain, “the final frontier—where it all goes in and comes out.” Her tools? A a recreated doctor’s office, an MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) brain scanner, a small wand with a tiny ceramic plate that heats up in calibrated increments to experimentally induce and study pain, and a dedicated team made up of two graduate students, two undergraduates and a full-time research coordinator.

Losin was provided with start-up money from the University, as relatively new faculty at UM, to set up her lab and gather initial data that can then be used to vie for follow-up funds from external granting agencies. Since 2015, she has been working with her team to understand the psychological and brain processes that are underlining our everyday social and cultural interactions and how these might influence the context of the pain experience.

“We’re trying to determine how social and cultural factors adjust the volume on people’s pain experiences,” she said, “because part of what we think is contributing to those disparities are social and cultural processes that range from experiences people have had throughout their lives, like discrimination, to experiences that they’re having acutely in the doctor’s office.”

One of the current studies in Losin’s lab enlists undergraduate students to play the parts of clinicians (pre-medical students) and patients. The closeness in age between these mock clinicians and patients helps reduce some of the variability of this factor in the analysis, and allows the researchers to hone in on contextual factors of the setting and the interaction between these two individuals.

“Most of the previous studies looking at demographic effects on pain are focused on the characteristics of the pain sufferer, the person in pain, but don’t address the context in which they’re experiencing the pain and talking about it to others,” said Losin. “We’re really focusing on those contextual effects and how they might modulate somebody’s report, and also potentially their internal experience.”

From this data, the researchers have already applied for federal funding to ask more nuanced pain perception and treatment questions of the human brain.  

“With this potential grant, we’d be looking at how clinicians perceive the pain of their patients and how that perception affects how they treat it,” said Losin. “What we’re trying to do is pit two potential contributors to these disparities against each other. One is the stereotypes clinicians hold, such as those about pain sensitivity of different groups or about drug abuse, for example.”

The more novel part of the grant, Losin added excitedly, is related to empathy at a very low, neural level. 

“This other potential contributor, which can only be accessed with brain imaging, is that clinicians have less pain-related brain activity in their own brains when looking at patients who don’t share their own demographic characteristics,” she said. “This phenomenon has been demonstrated repeatedly in the social neuroscience literature, but it has not really been applied to understanding pain treatment disparities.”

This recently submitted grant is aimed at addressing disparities in opioid prescribing.

“We’re trying to see, are these disparities mostly driven by top-down cognitive biases related to stereotypes, or is it really this bottom-up empathy bias at an unconscious, neural level? Or both? It’s probably both, but understanding the different mechanisms of these disparities can ultimately help us design more targeted interventions aimed at reducing them,” said Losin.

To develop strategies to reduce disparities in health and, specifically, pain, Losin champions for understanding these disparities at a foundational, mechanistic level in the brain, “the final frontier—where it all goes in and comes out,” she said. “If we don’t know what is going on in the brain, then it’s really hard to understand the behavior that emerges from it.”